In 1995,
Betty Hart, a professor of Human Development at the University of Kansas, and
Todd R. Risley, a professor of Psychology at the University of Alaska,
published a study
examining the difference in language heard by infants of varying socio-economic
backgrounds.
The study found
that the average low-income child heard
just over 600 words per hour, less than half the average total of working class
children (1251/hour), and less than one-third that of the average child in a
professional family (2153/hour). Using some rudimentary arithmetic, the
researchers projected that wealthy kids heard about 30 million words by age 3,
while poor kids heard only 10 million.
This
divergent led to what the researchers described as an “even-widening gap”
between poor and wealthy children, whereby the poor children not only “had smaller vocabularies than did
children of the same age in professional families, but they were also adding
words more slowly.”
More recent science also supports this
conclusion. A 2013 study
out of Stanford found that children in different socio-economic groups display dramatic
differences in their vocabularies by 18
months.
Thus, it should come as
little surprise that Head Start—the pioneering Great Society program designed
to improve early learning for poor children—has had limited success in closing
the achievement gap. As noted in a column
by UC-Berkeley Professor David Kirp this weekend, despite recent improvements
to the program, “a
2012 federal evaluation that used gold-standard methodology and concluded that
children who participated in Head Start were not more successful in elementary
school than others.”
Kirp goes on to argue that one of the
essential flaws in Head Start is that it only applies to poor students, in part
because branding Head Start as a program for the poor weakens its political
power, but more importantly because it concentrates the effects of poverty
rather than allowing poor students to interact with and learn from their better
educated peers. This interaction has been shown to help poor students narrow
the vocabulary/literacy gap with their more well to do contemporaries without
hurting the more privileged group.
While socioeconomic mixing in early childhood
education can help to mitigate the effects of the “word gap”, we need to do more to narrow/eliminate the
gap from emerging in the first place. This means placing a greater emphasis on
the formative years 0-3, as well as providing new parents with the skills and
tools they need to succeed. Simply put, Head Start is getting out of the
proverbial blocks too late for many students to catch up.
Creating children may come naturally to
humans, but parenting those children is anything but. And yet despite the
incredible importance and difficulty of parenthood, government offers little in the way of supports for
soon-to-be or new parents.
One program that has been effective in not only boosting pre-natal care
but in improving parenting practices, is Early
Head Start (EHS). Launched in 1995, EHS is designed to assist low-income
women and families on a variety of childhood development/parenting mattes. A
major study
of the program in 2005 found that EHS children “performed better than did
control children in cognitive and language development, displayed higher
emotional engagement of the parent and sustained attention with play objects,
and were lower in aggressive behavior. Compared with controls, Early Head Start
parents were more emotionally supportive, provided more language and learning
stimulation, read to their children more, and spanked less.”
[As
previously noted in this space,
Early Head Start or similar programs are perfect vehicles for Social Impact
Bond financing.]
This
conclusion isn’t surprising given that the vast majority of parents want to do well by their kids—they simply
need the tools to do so.
Indeed, the
fact that many parents do not read to their children as much as would be ideal
is not a “choice” in the traditional sense of the term. Not only are many
parents unaware of the benefits of frequent verbal interaction with infants,
but they also may lack the resources needed to simply have books/newspapers in
the home to read. For instance, parents who are given books and “prescriptions
for reading” by their children’s pediatricians have been found to be four
times more
likely to read and share books with their children.
This fall,
New York City will introduce universal pre-K for the first time. It’s a huge
step for equity and opportunity for our city’s youth and it is one of many bold
ideas that Mayor de Blasio is putting into motion. But even as we navigate the
challenges of pre-K, we should be planning for that next great leap forward in
early childhood education—to the formative years where the gap first emerges—so
that when the starting gun of life goes off, all children can get out of the
blocks.
No comments:
Post a Comment